Science & Technology
Aphantasia: why are some people unable to picture things in their mind?
May 20, 2023 ·
12 comments
Not everyone can picture something in their head. Image Credit: CC BY 2.0 Andrew Mason
The ability to mentally picture things might seem second nature, but some people are unable to do it at all.
When asked to close their eyes and imagine a sunset, most people can bring to mind an image of the sun setting on the horizon. Some people may experience more vivid details, such as vibrant colours, while others may produce a mental image that is blurry or lacks detail. But recent research has found that some people don't experience mental imagery at all.
This lack of mental imagery is called aphantasia. People with aphantasia are often surprised when they learn others see mental images in their minds. Many people with aphantasia have said they assumed others were speaking metaphorically when they described seeing something in their "mind's eye."
This is because our internal mental processes are not visible to others, so it is easy to assume everyone's minds operate in the same way. It is estimated that roughly four per cent of people have aphantasia.
People with aphantasia report a lack of visual imagery, and 97 per cent also report deficits in at least one other modality of imagery, like being unable to imagine certain sounds or tastes.
Everyday mental imagery
Mental imagery is involved in many other everyday cognitive processes. For example, visual and spatial imagery may play an important role in autobiographical memory. One study found that people with a tendency to generate vivid high-resolution mental images of objects and scenes can recall personal memories more quickly and in greater detail.
People with aphantasia are able to remember autobiographical facts, but when recalling life events they report fewer details and a less emotional experience, despite describing the events as important or personally relevant.
Mental imagery is also experienced when dreaming. Interestingly, 60 per cent of people with aphantasia report visual imagery in their dreams, although the quality of their experience is different from that of people with typical imagery ability. When dreaming, people with aphantasia report reduced experience across all senses, lower overall awareness and less sense of control.
The fact that people with aphantasia have some preserved mental imagery while dreaming suggests that aphantasia could be a deficit in voluntary mental imagery — the ability to deliberately bring images to mind — rather than involuntary mental imagery.
Imagery and reading
Many people also use mental imagery when reading. When we read, we create mental models to help make sense of the words and sentences, and research shows that we create these models using mental imagery.
For example, after reading a sentence such as "the ranger saw the eagle in the sky," people tend to be faster to recognize a picture of an eagle with outstretched wings, rather than a picture of an eagle sitting. This is because they visualize or simulate the situation described in the sentence, and this can help them quickly identify a picture that matches the imagined situation.
Aphantasia seems to affect whether or not people build mental models while they read. One study found that when reading descriptions of scary scenarios, people with aphantasia showed less emotional or physiological responses, suggesting their comprehension was less affected by imagined or simulated sensory experience.
These simulations can also occur subconsciously when we read single words and can help us access word meanings more quickly. For instance, when reading words related to sensory experiences such as vision, action and smell, the brain areas responsible for these senses are also active. However, there is currently no research on whether this is also true for people with aphantasia, so we don't know if they subconsciously simulate sensory or motor information when reading single words.
Different modes
People with aphantasia do not always consider their lack of mental imagery to be a negative thing. Some even credit their lack of imagery for success in other areas such as science and mathematics.
People with aphantasia also pursue creative vocations in fields such as visual art or writing, so mental imagery is not the same as imagination. In fact, the University of Glasgow curated an art exhibition featuring work by artists with aphantasia and those with hyperphantasia (extremely vivid mental imagery).
Discovering conditions like aphantasia tells us there may be different "modes" of cognition. For some people, thinking may involve simulating past sensory experiences both consciously and unconsciously. For others, and for people who have aphantasia, thinking may involve accessing facts.
Aphantasia shows us there is diversity in human cognition — despite our assumptions, our minds do not all work the same way. Research on aphantasia is just beginning, but it is a promising avenue to better understand the inner workings of the human mind.
Emiko Muraki, PhD Candidate, Brain & Cognitive Science,
University of Calgary and
Penny Pexman, Professor of Psychology,
University of Calgary
This article is republished from
The Conversation under a Creative Commons license.
Read the
original article.
Source:
The Conversation |
Comments (12)
Tags:
Aphantasia, Mind
Please Login or Register to post a comment.